Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Phase- and season-dependent changes in social behaviour in cyclic vole populations

Abstract

Background

Social behaviour has been linked to hypotheses explaining multiannual population cycles of small rodents. In this paper we aimed to test empirically that the degree of space sharing among adult breeding female voles is higher during the increase phase than in the crash phase, and that the degree of sociality is positively related to population growth rate as suggested by Lambin and Krebs (Oikos 61:126–132, 1991) and Andreassen et al. (Oikos 122:507–515, 2013). We followed 24 natural bank vole Myodes glareolus populations over an area of 113 km2 by monthly live trapping throughout a complete population cycle of three summers and two winters.

Results

Using spatially explicit capture-recapture models, we modelled the overlap in adult female home ranges and total population growth rate per season. We identified an increase phase before and during the peak density observation and a crash phase following the peak. Female home range overlap were seasonal- and phase-dependent, while population growth rate was associated with season and female home range overlap. High female home range overlap in the increase phase corresponded to a high population growth rate.

Conclusions

We suggest that intrinsic social behaviour plays a key role in the increase phase of vole population cycles, as social behaviour leads to an increased growth rate, whereas extrinsic factors (predation and/or food) initiate the crash phase. Our results are consistent with those of other studies in a variety of small rodent species.

Background

Social interactions linked to territoriality are among many factors contributing to the shaping of mammalian population dynamics [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8]. There are several definitions of territoriality [9], but it is most commonly defined as dominance through social interactions over a specific geographic area [10]. For small secretive species in which it is hard to determine whether a territory is actively defended or not, territoriality is generally defined as an exclusive use of space [11]. Regardless of whether active defence occurs or not, the result of territorial behaviour is the exclusive use of space, and hence territoriality is a factor which can limit population size.

Mammals show great flexibility in territorial behaviour. Within the same species there may be both territorial and non-territorial individuals, and the same individuals may even change between territorial and non-territorial behaviour [12,13,14]. This flexibility may be an adaptation to variations in the spatiotemporal environment in which the individuals live [15, 16]. For instance, social behaviour may provide benefits by increasing protection against predators, creating mating opportunities, reducing the risk of infanticide or by increasing success in finding and maintaining access to resources [2, 17]. At the same time, it can be costly due to increased competition for resources and mating opportunities, or increased risks of disease transmission or conspicuousness to predators [2, 18,19,20].

Social behaviour has been linked to many hypotheses, e.g. kin selection, explaining multiannual population cycles in the northern hemisphere [6, 7, 21,22,23,24,25]. Lambin and Krebs [7] described how changes in relatedness among female vole and lemming populations affected population growth rate; high degree of relatedness caused more overlap between female home ranges, higher reproduction and population peaks, while low relatedness lead to competition for territories and population declines. Andreassen et al. [5] provided a multifactorial model describing how the interaction between social behaviour (an intrinsic factor) and extrinsic factors (e.g. predation) could contribute to shaping the population dynamics of small rodents. In an individual based modelling exercise, it was later shown that this model could lead to population cycles with similar attributes (e.g. periodicity and amplitude) to those found in natural vole populations in Fennoscandia [26]. The model described phase-specific demographic responses associated with changes in sociality and extrinsic factors. The increase phase (1) consisted of highly stable social groups (possibly matriarchies as described by Lambin and Krebs [7]) with high survival and high reproductive rates. Amicable behaviour among normally territorial individuals was presumed to result from abundant, possibly patchily distributed, resources; the crash phase [2] was characterized by both a disruption of the social system and low recruitment and low survival. This disruption of the social system was expected to be mediated by increased predation rates, specifically of dominant individuals, followed by infanticide and increased movements of other individuals [5]. In particular it has been shown that social females have a higher mortality rate if they are exposed to the infanticide of pups [27]. Hence, the model suggests that the increase phase should be characterized by less territorial behaviour than the crash phase.

In the bank vole, Myodes glareolus, adult females have mutually exclusive home ranges [28], whereas adult males have large home ranges that overlap extensively [29]. It has been hypothesized that food availability is one of the main causes of territoriality in female small rodents [13, 30, 31]. Females relying on sparse and slowly renewed food resources are expected to defend territories covering important food patches [11]. Alternatively, it has been suggested that females defend territories to prevent infanticide [12, 32,33,34]. However, territoriality of female bank voles is quite flexible. Rémy et al. [35] found that a clumped and highly predictable food source increased sociality, which led to a positive effect on the population growth rate through higher success in producing weaned offspring. Other studies have shown that bank voles are less territorial during winter [36], when communal living is expected to be beneficial due to enhanced thermoregulation [37]. In this study, we monitored up to 24 natural bank vole populations monthly for over 2 years, through the increase and crash phase of a population cycle, to empirically test the assumed phase dependent changes in sociality of the multifactorial model of Andreassen et al. [5] and the kin selection model by Lambin and Krebs [7]. We used female home range overlap as a proxy for sociality, where a higher degree of home range overlap among females indicates a higher degree of social tolerance. We predicted an association between social behaviour and the phase of the population cycle, with greater sociality, i.e. higher home range overlap, among female bank voles during the increase phase than during the crash phase.

Materials and methods

Study area

The study took place from June 2013 to August 2015 in Stor-Elvdal municipality, Southeast Norway (61°N, 11°E). The study area was a typical boreal forest dominated by Norway spruce (Picea abies) and Scotch pine (Pinus sylvestris), with bilberry (Vaccinium myrtillus) in the field layer and mosses (e.g. Pleurozium schreberi) in the ground layer. The climate was continental with relatively dry weather, large diurnal variation in temperature, warm summers and cold winters [38]. Snow cover normally lasts from December to April. The region experienced peaks in vole population density during the breeding seasons of the years 2007, 2011, 2014 and 2017 (Current study, and unpublished data from Inland Norway University of Applied Sciences).

Trapping procedure

Voles were caught on 60 m × 60 m grids consisting of 16 Ugglan multiple capture live traps (Granab, Sweden) arranged in a cross pattern, with 15 m spacing (Fig. 1a), along three elevation transects (Mykleby, Gåla and Evenstad). The transects ranged from 267 to 801 meters above sea level (mean altitude Mykleby: 537 masl., Gåla: 662 masl., Evenstad: 429 masl.). Each transect consisted of 8 trapping grids (n = 24). Grids were separated by at least 500 m. They were located in typical bank vole habitat, in mature forest areas with a bilberry-dominated field layer [39, 40]. Where cross-shaped grids did not encompass suitable habitat (n = 5), grids were arranged in a linear shape with 9 to 12 traps (Fig. 1b). Traps were placed in runways or close to holes with potential vole activity. All traps were marked with a stick and a ribbon in the closest tree and remained in the same place throughout the study. Between trapping sessions, voles could use the traps as part of their runway system. During winter, each trap was placed inside a plywood-box (30 cm × 30 cm × 40 cm) with no floor, making it possible for voles to move freely in and out of the box, and with a lid to prevent the traps from being covered with snow. The boxes were removed in spring when the snow melted around them. During each monthly trapping session capture-recapture trapping was carried out for three nights, i.e. traps were checked every morning and evening yielding a total of 6 secondary trapping sessions. We trapped from June 2013 to August 2015, with the exception of September 2014. During winter we reduced the number of trapping grids to 14 due to more time consuming trapping in winter. We also lost some trap nights due to extreme cold (− 20 °C), and some trapping grids due to heavy snow concealing the traps (Table 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Trapping grid design. a Shows the main, cross-shaped design with 16 traps, and b the alternative design used when the main design did not encompass any suitable vole habitat, with 12 traps

Table 1 Trapping history

We baited the traps with oats and carrots, and we added sawdust in the cold period to absorb urine, preventing it from freezing onto the voles, and to help keep them warm. All voles were individually marked with a pit-tag (1.25 × 7 mm ID-100VB Nano Transponder) and sexed, weighed to the nearest gram and checked for reproductive status (mature if open vagina in females and scrotal testicles in males). We used a basic LID-560 Pocket Reader to read previous tags.

Data analyses

Season and population phases

Our study consisted of trapping data from a complete population cycle, including an increase, peak, crash and low phase. We used the minimum number of bank voles known to be alive (MNKA) to give a preliminary monthly description of the population trajectory (Fig. 2) which we then used to identify the cyclic phases. MNKA gives us a more detailed (monthly) population trajectory than the density estimates (described below) gives us. Both the summers of 2013 and 2014 had high population densities with a winter decrease in between. Hence, we chose to consider the whole June 2013–August 2014 period as the increase phase, with a peak in August 2014. We considered the period starting in September 2014 as the crash phase of the cycle. The population crashed and reached the low phase in late winter 2014/2015.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Top: a simulation with three peak years illustrating the cycle phase in the present paper in blue. Bottom: minimum number known to be alive (MNKA) per trap per month from June 2013 to August 2015 (missing trapping in September 2014 denoted with NA). We used these estimates to define the increase and decrease phases of the cycle for further analyses. Seasons are shown with colours: blue = summer, orange = fall, white = winter, red = spring

We defined the seasons as summer: June–August; fall: September–November; winter: December–February; and spring: March–May (Fig. 2).

Spatially explicit capture-recapture models

Spatially explicit capture-recapture (SECR) models assume that each individual has an activity centre, and that the capture probability is a function of the distance between the trap and the individual’s activity centre. We estimated the density (D) and the spatial scale of detection sigma (σ) for each trapping grid and season using SECR models [41, 42]. We fitted the models using the secr package [43] in the statistical software R [44]. The parameter σ describes the relationship between the detection probability (g0) and the distance between a trap and an animal activity centre; it can be understood as a metric of home range size, where the spatial scale of detection (σ) increases with the home range size [8, 41]. To estimate the detection probability we used a half-normal detection function and a spatial buffer of 50 m. The model parameters (D, g0, σ) were set to be dependent on season rather than month to obtain long enough trapping histories for the SECR-modelling. We had too few recaptures to include any covariates in the models, and consequently there were no need for model selection.

Efford et al. [41] suggested re-parameterizing the models using k = σ √D, which is an expression of home range overlap between individuals. Consequently, if we assume σ to be a circular bivariate normal home range, it is possible to estimate the number of individuals within a certain percentage isopleth of the home range. The relationship between home range size and density can then be expressed as σ = k/√D or as a linear regression equation: log(σ) = log(k) + β log(√D). If overlap is constant among populations [i.e. the intercept log(k)], the slope β is assumed to be − 1/2 as home range size is halved when density is doubled [41].

We were not able to estimate k for adult females at the grid level, because the female population size was too low. We therefore ran the linear regression described above between log(σ) and density, where we weighted sigma by 1/SE2 [45], and obtained the residuals. We used these residuals as an index of home range overlap, with negative (or positive) values indicating less (or more) overlap than expected from the population density. After November 2014 there were too few captures to estimate any parameter of the population.

We estimated home range overlap among adult females only as we were interested in the changes in sociality in the territorial part of the population, while we estimated population density for the whole trappable part of the population.

Linear mixed models

We analysed the factors influencing home range overlap with linear mixed models using phase and season as fixed effects and trapping grid as a random effect. We calculated the seasonal population growth rate (Dt/Dt−1) where t was the 3-month seasonal period, and analysed it using linear mixed models with phase, season and female home range overlap as fixed effects and trapping grid as the random effect.

We performed model selection based on Akaike’s information criterion corrected for small sample sizes (AICc). All analyses were done using R [44], with the package lme4 [46].

Results

There was both a seasonal- and phase-dependent effect on female home range overlap, as the best model (Female home range overlap = f (phase + season)) accounted for 99% of the AICc-weights (Table 2). Female density did not decrease dramatically in the beginning of the crash phase (Fig. 3a). Female home range overlap was highest in the first summer/fall of the increase phase, i.e. in 2013, somewhat lower during the second increase year and lowest during the crash phase, in the fall of 2014 (Fig. 3b). The low home range overlap in fall 2014 was not due to the estimates being based on only 2 months’ data as including December in the fall 2014 estimates did not change it (mean residual home range overlap ± SE for Oct–Nov fall 2014 = − 0.22 ± 0.07, for Oct–Dec fall 2014 = − 0.25 ± 0.08).

Table 2 Model selection results of linear mixed model explaining female bank vole home range overlap
Fig. 3
figure 3

Female density per hectar (mean ± 2SE) (a), residual female home range overlap (mean ± 2SE) (b), and population growth rate (mean ± 2SE) (c) across seasons from summer 2013 (June–August) to Winter 2014–2015 (December–February). Due to too few captures, we were not able to estimate female density and consequently home range overlap after November 2014

There were effects of season and female home range overlap on the population growth rate (Table 3). Population growth rates were highest during the increase phase in spring and summer. However, growth rates decreased between summer and fall 2014 (Fig. 3c). Growth rates were low in fall and winter during both the increase and crash phases.

Table 3 Model selection results of linear mixed model explaining growth rate in bank voles

There was a correlation between female home range overlap and population growth rate (rspear = 0.6). Generally, low female home range overlap corresponded with low population growth rates (Fig. 4). In the first fall, female home range overlap was high and associated with a population that had not yet crashed, even though the growth rate had dropped to a seasonal-dependent low.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Correlation between population growth rate (mean ± 2SE) and residual female home range overlap (mean ± 2SE). Summer 13 = June–August 13, Fall 13 = September–November 13, Winter 13/14 = December–February 13/14, Spring 14 = March–May 14, Summer 14 = June–August 14, Fall 14 = October–November 14. Grey represent the increase phase of the cycle and blue represents the peak crash of the cycle

Discussion

Following a couple of decades without any high density peaks in Scandinavian vole populations [47], regular peaks, characteristic of cyclic vole populations, have occurred in the study area since 2007. In this study we were able to observe in detail a complete cycle including the increase and crash phase. The crash was observed as the extinction of trappable bank voles in our grids in May 2015. We analysed the data according to the framework for cyclic vole populations proposed by Andreassen et al. [5], in which phase-dependent social change was linked to population demography. We showed that population growth rate was associated with both season and female home range overlap, while female home range overlap was season- and phase-dependent. The increase phase was characterized by year round high female home range overlap and high population growth rates in spring/summer. The crash phase began when population density was still high but female home range overlap and population growth rate were low.

We chose to define two phases of the population cycle: an increase phase preceding and including the peak density observation in 2014, and a crash phase following the peak observation. Consequently there were two breeding seasons in the increase phase, with a non-breeding winter in-between during which the population density decreased. We did not consider this decrease as a crash, so much as a natural seasonal fluctuation in mortality combined with a lack of breeding. Most vole populations have been studied by trapping once or twice a year [47, 48], which does not allow such details in the population trajectories to be revealed. Obviously, high or peak vole densities, may occur 2 years in a row [49], which would extend the functional definition of the increase phase to 1–2 years, with natural seasonal fluctuations in densities. In contrast, we observed a crash phase characterized by a more consistent decrease in densities in fall/winter, which extended into the breeding season (summer 2015) and ended with the extinction of trappable animals. This is the typical crash phase of cyclic vole populations according to Hansson and Henttonen [50].

As predicted by Andreassen et al. [5] and Lambin and Krebs [7], adult females were most social during the increase phase, i.e. the female home range overlap were highest in this phase. Andreassen et al. [5] suggest that the individuals present at the start of the increase phase had survived the bottleneck of the previous cycle, i.e. the winter of the previous low phase, and that these individuals were presumed to be patchily located in high quality, core habitats [51]. Patchiness of resources is suggested to trigger the formation of groups in social females [13], leading to a higher breeding success and a higher population growth rate [35, 52]. In such a situation, the degree of kinship within the group could have a positive effect on recruitment and survival [7, 53, 54]. The more amicable behaviour of territorial females during the increase phase of the cycle may ultimately be due to kin structure [7, 55] and/or an adaptation to ample resources, thus making it more rewarding to use energy to survive and reproduce than to defend a territory against intruders [56].

Previous studies proposed that territorial behaviour should be more relaxed during the non-breeding season [57,58,59], and that winter aggregations and nest sharing were adaptations that may increase winter survival [60] by enhancing thermoregulation [37]. Although there was a large variation in female home range overlap during winter in our study, the mean female home range overlap decreased from fall to winter, indicating a higher degree of territoriality during winter. Food availability is an important factor affecting territoriality in females [13, 30, 31], and we know that it can be a limiting factor during winter [61, 62]. West and Dublin [60] proposed that food scarcity was the only reason to maintain territoriality in winter. Therefore, we suggest that in our study area territoriality is at least as strong in winter as during the breeding season due to the defence of possibly scarce and low-quality food resources.

Female home range overlap and population growth rate decreased dramatically from summer to fall in 2014, going from the increase to the crash phase, while the minimum number of voles known to be alive and the number of adult females remained quite high in fall 2014. This corresponded to the predictions of the framework described by Andreassen et al. [5] for a crash phase where female social system changes before estimates of abundance. In their framework the crash was initiated by predation, and that predation increased as a result of predators’ functional and numerical responses to high vole densities [63]. Further the framework suggests that as dominant males move over large areas and make risky movements to access females, they are more exposed to predation and consequently are the first individuals to be predated [64]. If the dominant males disappear, immigration of new males to vacant territories may reduce recruitment and the population growth rate due to infanticide [27, 32]. Although density can still be high at the beginning of this stage, the social system has been disrupted and contribute to further decrease in densities. Then the framework describes that females that have been exposed to infanticide and have lost their pups, tend to start moving around and become increasingly exposed to predation. In this situation, a more aggressive and territorial behaviour may be an adaptation to avoid infanticide, hostile individuals and predation. These intrinsic and extrinsic factors may lead to a population crash according to the framework described by Andreassen et al. [5]. In an individual-based model system, Radchuk et al. [26] confirmed that this combination of extrinsic (predation) and intrinsic (sociality and dispersal) factors could shape population cycles.

Alternatively, a crash could be initiated by limited food resources as a result of overexploitation during two high density summers. Bank voles have been found to lose weight and die when feeding on a green diet alone [65], so they may depend on specific high quality food resources for growth and survival (see also [66]). A shortage of seeds, for instance, could force adult females to become more territorial during the crash phase [11], as observed in our study. Home range overlap was lower during the crash phase than during the peak phase in the mast-induced cyclic yellow-necked mouse Apodemus flavicollis [8].

Conclusions

Although the generality of the association we observed between changes in territorial behaviour and phase of the population cycles may be questioned because it is based on only one population cycle, our observations came from 24 populations across 3 transects covering a large area (113 km2 area between the most distant grids). Moreover, an association between social structure and population growth rates has been described for several species of rodents from the Cricetidae family, including the house mouse (Mus spp.) [67, 68], yellow-necked mouse [8], and Myodes and Microtus voles (see Andreassen et al. [5] and references therein). These species inhabit a variety of biomes and show population dynamics that vary from occasional outbreaks to more regular population peaks characteristics of population cycles. In all these situations, amicable behaviour has been associated with the increase phase of population density, often related to ample resources, while the decrease or crash phase of the fluctuations has been associated with disruption of the social system and a higher degree of territoriality. In contrast, the evidence that food, predation and/or kin structure shape changes in social behaviour, and in turn population dynamics, is equivocal.

References

  1. Odden M, Ims RA, Støen OG, Swenson JE, Andreassen HP. Bears are simply voles writ large: social structure determines the mechanisms of intrinsic population regulation in mammals. Oecologia. 2014;175:1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Silk JB. The adaptive value of sociality in mammalian groups. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2007;362:539–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Milner JM, Nilsen EB, Andreassen HP. Demographic side effects of selective hunting in ungulates and carnivores. Conserv Biol. 2007;21:36–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Rutledge LY, Patterson BR, Mills KJ, Loveless KM, Murray DL, White BN. Protection from harvesting restores the natural social structure of eastern wolf packs. Biol Conserv. 2010;143:332–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Andreassen HP, Glorvigen P, Rémy A, Ims RA. New views on how population-intrinsic and community-extrinsic processes interact during the vole population cycles. Oikos. 2013;122:507–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Charnov EL, Finerty JP. Vole population cycles: a case for kin-selection? Oecologia. 1980;45:1–2.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Lambin X, Krebs CJ. Can changes in female relatedness influence microtine population dynamics? Oikos. 1991;61:126–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Bogdziewicz M, Zwolak R, Redosh L, Rychlik L, Crone EE. Negative effects of density on space use of small mammals differ with the phase of the masting-induced population cycle. Ecol Evol. 2016;6:8423–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Maher CR, Lott DF. Definitions of territoriality used in the study of variation in vertebrate spacing systems. Anim Behav. 1995;49:1581–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Kaufmann JH. On the definitions and functions of dominance and territoriality. Biol Rev. 1983;58:1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Ostfeld RS. The ecology of territoriality in small mammals. Trends Ecol Evol. 1990;5:411.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Wolff JO. Why are female small mammals territorial? Oikos. 1993;68:364–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Ims RA. Responses in spatial organization and behaviour to manipulations of the food resource in the vole Clethrionomys rufocanus. J Anim Ecol. 1987;56:585–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Ims RA. Spatial clumping of sexually receptive females induces space sharing. Nature. 1988;335:541–3.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Erlinge S, Sandell M. Seasonal changes in the social organization of male stoats, Mustela erminea: an effect of shifts between two decisive resources. Oikos. 1986;47:57–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Fretwell SD, Lucas HLJ. On territorial behaviour and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds. Acta Biotheor. 1969;19:16–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Schoepf I, Schmohl G, König B, Pillay N, Schradin C. Manipulation of population density and food availability affects home range sizes of African striped mouse females. Anim Behav. 2015;99:53–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Lambin X, Krebs CJ. Influence of female relatedness on the demography of townsend’s vole populations in spring. J Anim Ecol. 1993;62:536–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Pedersen AB, Greives TJ. The interaction of parasites and resources cause crashes in a wild mouse population. J Anim Ecol. 2008;77:370–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Wright AN, Gompper ME. Altered parasite assemblages in raccoons in response to manipulated resource availability. Oecologia. 2005;144:148–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Krebs CJ. Population cycles revicited. J Mammal. 1996;77:8–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Boonstra R, Boag PT. Spring declines in Microtus pennsylvanicus and the role of steroid hormones. J Anim Ecol. 1992;61:339–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Boonstra R, Hochachka WM. Maternal effects and additive genetic inheritance in the collared lemming Dicrostonyx groenlandicus. Evol Ecol. 1997;11:169–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Piertney SB, Lambin X, Maccoll ADC, Lock K, Bacon PJ, Dallas JF, Leckie F, Mougeot F, Racey PA, Redpath S, Moss R. Temporal changes in kinstructure through a population cycle in a territorial bird, the red grouse Lagopus lagopus scoticus. Mol Ecol. 2008;17:2544–51.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Matthiopoulos J, Moss R, Lambin X. The kin facilitation hypothesis for red grouse population cycles: territorial dynamics of the family cluster. Ecol Mod. 2002;147:291–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Radchuk V, Ims RA, Andreassen HP. From individuals to population cycles: the role of extrinsic and intrinsic factors in rodent populations. Ecology. 2016;97:720–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Andreassen HP, Gundersen G. Male turnover reduces population growth: an encolsure experiment on voles. Ecology. 2006;87:88–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Bujalska G. Reprodction stabilizing elements in an island population of Clethrionomys glareolus. Acta Theriol (Warsz). 1970;25:381–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Mazurkiewicz M. Shape, size and distribution of home ranges of Clethrionomys glareolus. Acta Theriol (Warsz). 1971;XVI:23–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Ostfeld RS. Limiting resources and territoriality in microtine rodents. Am Nat. 1985;126:1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Gray SJ, Jensen SP, Hurst JL. Effects of resource distribution on activity and territory defence in house mice. Mus domesticus. Anim Behav. 2002;63:531–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Ylönen H, Koskela E, Mappes T. Infanticide in the bank vole (Clethrionomys glareolus): occurrence and the effect of familiarity on female infanticide. Ann Zool Fennici. 1997;34:259–66.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Wolff JO, Peterson JA. An offspring-defense hypothesis for territoriality in female mammals. Ethol Ecol Evol. 1998;10:227–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Festa-Bianchet M, Boag DA. Territoriality in adult female Columbian ground squirrels. Can J Zool. 1982;60:1060–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Rémy A, Odden M, Richard M, Stene MT, Le Galliard JF, Andreassen HP. Food distribution influences social organization and population growth in a small rodent. Behav Ecol. 2013;24:832–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Bujalska G. Social system of the bank vole, Clethrionomys glareolus. In: Tamarin RH, Ostfeld RS, Pugh SR, Bujalska G, editors. Social systems and population cycles in voles. Basel: Birkhauser Verlag; 1990. p. 155–67.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  37. Vickery WL, Millar JS. The energetics og huddling by endoterms. Oikos. 1984;43:88–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Boonstra R, Andreassen HP, Boutin S, Hušek J, Ims RA, Krebs CJ, et al. Why do the boreal forest ecosystems of Northwestern Europe differ from those of Western North America? Bioscience. 2016;66:722–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Myllymäki A. Interactions between the field vole Microtus agrestis and its microtine competitors in Central-Scandinavian populations. Oikos. 1977;29:570–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Gorini L, Linnell JDC, Boitani L, Hauptmann U, Odden M, Wegge P, et al. Guild composition and habitat use of voles in 2 forest landscapes in south-eastern Norway. Integr Zool. 2011;6:299–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Efford MG, Dawson DK, Jhala YV, Qureshi Q. Density-dependent home-range size revealed by spatially explicit capture-recapture. Ecography. 2016;39:676–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Efford MG. Estimation of population density by spatially explicit capture—recapture analysis of data from area searches. Ecology. 2011;92:2202–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Efford MG. secr: Spatially explicit capture-recapture models; 2016. https://cran.r-project.org/package=secr. Accessed 22 Sept 2016.

  44. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Sheather SJ. Weighted least squares. In: Casella G, Fienberg S, Olkin I, editors. A modern approach to regression with R. New York: Springer; 2009. p. 115–22.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  46. Bates D, Maechler M. lme4: linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375-31; 2009. http://cran.r-project.org. Accessed 1 May 2016.

  47. Hörnfeldt B. Long-term decline in numbers of cyclic voles in boreal Sweden: analysis and presentation of hypotheses. Oikos. 2004;107:376–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Cornulier T, Yoccoz NG, Bretagnolle V, Brommer JE, Butet A, Ecke F, et al. Europe-wide dampening of population cycles in keystone herbivores. Science. 2013;340:63–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Krebs CJ, Myers JH. Population cycles in small mammals. Adv Ecol Res. 1974;8:268–389.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Hansson L, Henttonen H. Rodent dynamics as community process. Trends Ecol Evol. 1988;3:195–200.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Sundell J, Church C, Ovaskainen O. Spatio-temporal patterns of habitat use in voles and shrews modified by density, season and predators. J Anim Ecol. 2012;81:747–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Ylönen H, Viitala J. Social overwintering and food distribution in the bank vole Clethrionomys glareolus. Holarct Ecol. 1991;14:131–7.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Ylönen H, Mappes T, Viitala J. Different demography of friends and strangers: an experiment on the impact of kinship and familiarity in Clethrionomys glareolus. Oecologia. 1990;83:333–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Mappes T, Ylönen H, Viitala J. Higher reproductive success among kin groups of bank voles (Clethrionomys glareolus). Ecology. 1995;76:1276–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Lambin X, Yoccoz NG. The impact of population kin-structure on nestling survival in Townsend’s voles Microtus townsendii. J Anim Ecol. 1998;68:1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Davies NB. Ecological questions about territorial behaviour. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB, editors. Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. 1st ed. Oxford: Blackwell Science; 1978. p. 317–50.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Eccard JA, Ylönen H. Interspecific competition in small rodents: from populations to individuals. Evol Ecol. 2003;17:423–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Karlsson F, Ås S. The use of winter home ranges in a low density Clethrionomys glareolus population. Oikos. 1987;50:213–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Perrin MR. The roles of reproduction, survival, and territoriality in the seasonal dynamics of Clethrionomys gapperi populations. Acta Oecol. 1979;24:475–500.

    Google Scholar 

  60. West SD, Dublin HT. Behavioral strategies of small mammals under winter conditions: solitary or social? In: Merrit JF, editor. Winter ecology of small mammals. 1st ed. Pittsburgh: Carnegie Museum of Natural History; 1984. p. 293–9.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Johnsen K, Boonstra R, Boutin S, Devineau O, Krebs CJ, Andreassen HP. Surviving winter: food, but not habitat structure, prevents crashes in cyclic vole populations. Ecol Evol. 2017;7:115–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Huitu O, Koivula M, Korpimäki E, Klemola T, Norrdahl K. Winter food supply limits growth of northern vole populations in the absence of predation. Ecology. 2003;84:2108–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Sundell J. Experimental tests of the role of predation in the population. Mamm Rev. 2006;36:107–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Haapakoski M, Ylönen H. Effects of fragmented breeding habitat and resource distribution on behavior and survival of the bank vole (Myodes glareolus). Popul Ecol. 2010;52:427–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Kostelecka-Myrcha A, Myrcha A. The rate of passage of foodstuffs through the alimentary tracts of certain Microtidae under laboratory conditions. Acta Theriol (Warsz). 1964;9:37–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Andreassen HP, Bondrup-Nielsen S. A comparisonof the effects of a moss diet and a varied diet on the growth of juvenile wood lemmings, Myopus schisticolor (Lilljeb.). Zeitschrift fur Saügetierkd. 1991;56:378–9.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Krebs CJ, Chitty D, Singleton GR, Boonstra R. Can changes in social behaviour help to explain house mouse plagues in Australia? Oikos. 1995;73:429–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Sutherland DR, Spencer PBS, Singleton GR, Taylor AC. Kin interactions and changing social structure during a population outbreak of feral house mice. Mol Ecol. 2005;14:2803–14.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors’ contributions

KJ has collected data, led analyses and wrote first draft of the manuscript. OD has contributed to analyses, intellectual input and comments. HPA designed the study, contributed with intellectual input, analyses, writing and comments. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all students helping us with the field work, and a special thanks to Barbara Joncour, David Carricondo Sanchez and Thomas Vogler.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Availability of data

The datasets analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Scientific permit to conduct live-trapping and pit-tagging of voles was approved by the Norwegian food safety authority (FOTS ID 5525) according to Norwegian law.

Funding

This study is a part of the BEcoDyn project supported by Inland Norway University of Applied Sciences and a grant from the Norwegian Research Council (NFR project 221056) to Harry P. Andreassen. The Norwegian Research Council did not take a part in any of the stages of this study except the funding.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kaja Johnsen.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Johnsen, K., Devineau, O. & Andreassen, H.P. Phase- and season-dependent changes in social behaviour in cyclic vole populations. BMC Ecol 19, 5 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12898-019-0222-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12898-019-0222-3

Keywords